Research in Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
ISSN (Print): 2333-4371 ISSN (Online): 2333-438X Website: https://www.sciepub.com/journal/rpbs Editor-in-chief: Apply for this position
Open Access
Journal Browser
Go
Research in Psychology and Behavioral Sciences. 2016, 4(1), 1-6
DOI: 10.12691/rpbs-4-1-1
Open AccessArticle

The Effect of Predisposing Risk Factors of an Eating Disorder on Response Inhibition and Working Memory: An Event-Related Potentials Study

Alison Osborne1 and Leigh M. Riby1,

1Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle Upon Tyne, UK

Pub. Date: January 19, 2016

Cite this paper:
Alison Osborne and Leigh M. Riby. The Effect of Predisposing Risk Factors of an Eating Disorder on Response Inhibition and Working Memory: An Event-Related Potentials Study. Research in Psychology and Behavioral Sciences. 2016; 4(1):1-6. doi: 10.12691/rpbs-4-1-1

Abstract

A novel investigation was undertaken to assess the effect of a predisposition to an eating disorder on P3a and P3b event-related potentials. Previous research has suggested the P3a and the P3b are reliable markers of inhibitory control and working memory updating, respectively. An opportunity sample of 12 female participants was obtained with mean age of 22.42 (SD = 2.61). Participants completed the Eating Disorder Inventory – 3 assessing their predisposition to an eating disorder along with scores on the included subscales. Response inhibition and working memory was measured using the 3 Stimulus Oddball Task. This task elicits a P3a component in response to novel infrequently presented stimuli and the P3b component in the response to expected infrequent stimuli. Findings showed no evidence of an interaction between an overall predisposition to an eating disorder and P3a and P3b activations. However, results ascertained a significant positive correlation between Body Dissatisfaction scores and the P3a amplitudes. Individuals with a high score on body dissatisfaction scale showed greater activation towards the frontal region than those with a low score during the executive component task, i.e. the greater the score the greater hyperactivity in the frontal area of the brain during response inhibition. With regards to the working memory component, no significant effects were found. Although head maps for body dissatisfaction scores and working memory illustrated that there was a wider spread of activation for the high body dissatisfaction group, rather than concentrated activations in the parietal region.The implications of such results in respect to compensatory activations, the inability to ignore and possible dopamine involvement are discussed.

Keywords:
eating disorder body dissatisfaction EEG ERPP3a P3boddball task P300 dopamine inhibition executive function

Creative CommonsThis work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

References:

[1]  Bemporad, J.R, “Cultural and historical aspects of eating disorders,” Theoretical Medicine, 18(4). 401-420. 1997.
 
[2]  Garner, D.M, EDI 3: Eating Disorder Inventory-3: Professional Manual, Psychological Assessment Resources. 2004.
 
[3]  American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders(5th ed.), American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, VA. 2013.
 
[4]  Corstorphine, E, “Cognitive–emotional–behavioural therapy for the eating disorders: Working with beliefs about emotions,” European Eating Disorders Review, 14(6). 448-461. Oct.2006
 
[5]  McKnight Investigators, “Risk factors for the onset of eating disorders in adolescent girls: results of the McKnight longitudinal risk factor study,” American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(2). 248-254. Feb.2003.
 
[6]  Obeid, N., Buchholz, A., Boerner, K.E., Henderson, K.A., and Norris, M, “Self-esteem and social anxiety in an adolescent female eating disorder population: age and diagnostic effects,” Eating disorders,21(2). 140-153. Feb.2013
 
[7]  Stice, E., Davis, K., Miller, N.P., and Marti, C.N, “Fasting increases risk for onset of binge eating and bulimic pathology: a 5-year prospective study,” Journal of abnormal psychology, 117(4). 941. Nov.2008.
 
[8]  Fairburn, C.G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H.A., and Davies, B.A, “Identifying dieters who will develop an eating disorder: a prospective, population-based study,” American Journal of Psychiatry. Dec.2014.
 
[9]  Shafran, R., Cooper, Z. and Fairburn, C.G, “Clinical perfectionism” is not “multidimensional perfectionism: a reply to Hewitt, Flett, Besser, Sherry and McGee,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41. 1217-1220. 2003
 
[10]  Stice, E, “A prospective test of the dual-pathway model of bulimic pathology: mediating effects of dieting and negative affect,” Journal of abnormal psychology, 110(1). 124. Feb.2001.
 
[11]  Stice, E., and Shaw, H.E, “Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: A synthesis of research findings,” Journal of psychosomatic research, 53(5). 985-993. Nov.2002.
 
[12]  Garner, D.M., and Bemis, K.M, “A cognitive-behavioral approach to anorexia nervosa,” Cognitive therapy and research, 6(2). 123-150. Jun.1982.
 
[13]  Kothari, R., Solmi, F., Treasure, J., and Micali, N, “The neuropsychological profile of children at high risk of developing an eating disorder,” Psychological medicine, 43(07). 1543-1554. Jul.2013.
 
[14]  Parrish, J., Geary, E., Jones, J., Seth, R., Hermann, B., and Seidenberg, M, “Executive functioning in childhood epilepsy: parent‐report and cognitive assessment,” Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology,49(6). 412-416. Jun.2007.
 
[15]  Riccio, C.A., and Gomes, H, “Interventions for executive function deficits in children and adolescents,” Applied Neuropsychology: Child,2(2). 133-140. Jan.2013.
 
[16]  Dobson, K.S., and Dozois, D.J, “Attentional biases in eating disorders: A meta-analytic review of Stroop performance,” Clinical Psychology Review, 23(8). 1001-1022. Jan.2004.
 
[17]  Tchanturia, K., Davies, H., Roberts, M., Harrison, A., Nakazato, M., Schmidt, U., Treasure, J., and Morris, R, “Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: examining the evidence using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task,” PLoS One, 7(1). Jan.2012.
 
[18]  Galimberti, E., Martoni, R.M., Cavallini, M.C., Erzegovesi, S., and Bellodi, L, “Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in eating disorder subtypes,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 36(2). 307-312. Mar.2012.
 
[19]  Kemps, E., Tiggemann, M., Wade, T., Ben‐Tovim, D., and Breyer, R, “Selective working memory deficits in anorexia nervosa,” European Eating Disorders Review,14(2). 97-103. Mar.2006.
 
[20]  Scarano, G.M., and Kalodner‐Martin, C.R, “A description of the continuum of eating disorders: Implications for intervention and research,” Journal of Counseling & Development,72(4). 356-361. Apr.1994.
 
[21]  Williamson, D.A., Womble, L.G., Smeets, M.A., Netemeyer, R.G., Thaw, J.M., Kutlesic, V., and Gleaves, D.H, “Latent structure of eating disorder symptoms: A factor analytic and taxometric investigation,” American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(3). 412-418. Mar.2002.
 
[22]  Simmonds, D.J., Pekar, J.J., and Mostofsky, S.H, “Meta-analysis of Go/No-go tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task-dependent,” Neuropsychologia, 46(1). 224-232. Jan.2008.
 
[23]  Marsh, R., Maia, T.V., and Peterson, B.S, “Functional disturbances within frontostriatal circuits across multiple childhood psychopathologies,” The American journal of psychiatry, 166(6). 664-674. Jun.2009.
 
[24]  Zastrow, A., Kaiser, S., Stippich, C., Walther, S., Herzog, W., Tchanturia, K., and Friederich, H.C, “Neural correlates of impaired cognitive-behavioral flexibility in anorexia nervosa,” American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(5). 608-616. May.2009.
 
[25]  Baddeley, A.D., and Hitch, G, “Working memory,” Psychology of learning and motivation, 8. 47-89. 1974.
 
[26]  Baddeley, A, “The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?” Trends in cognitive sciences, 4(11). 417-423. Oct.2000.
 
[27]  Brooks, S.J., Owen, G.O., Uher, R., Friederich, H.C., Giampietro, V., Brammer, M., and Campbell, I.C, “Differential neural responses to food images in women with bulimia versus anorexia nervosa,” PLoS One,6(7). Jul.2010.
 
[28]  Kullmann, S., Giel, K.E., Hu, X., Bischoff, S.C., Teufel, M., Thiel, A., and Preissl, H, “Impaired inhibitory control in anorexia nervosa elicited by physical activity stimuli,” Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 9(7). 917-923. Jan.2014.
 
[29]  Pessoa, L., Gutierrez, E., Bandettini, P.A., and Ungerleider, L.G, “Neural correlates of visual working memory: fMRI amplitude predicts task performance,” Neuron,35(5). 975-987. Aug.2002.
 
[30]  Castro-Fornieles, J., Caldú, X., Andrés-Perpiñá, S., Lázaro, L., Bargalló, N., Falcón, C., and Junqué, C, “A cross-sectional and follow-up functional MRI study with a working memory task in adolescent anorexia nervosa,” Neuropsychologia,48(14). 4111-4116. Dec.2010.
 
[31]  Fairburn, C.G., and Beglin, S.J, “Assessment of eating disorders: Interview or self‐report questionnaire?,” International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16(4). 363-370. Dec.1994.
 
[32]  Fenton, O., and Ecker, U.K, “Memory updating in sub-clinical eating disorder: Differential effects with food and body shape words,” Eating behaviors, 17. 103-106. Apr.2015.
 
[33]  Polich, J, Theoretical overview of P3a and P3b, Springer, US, 2003, 83-98.
 
[34]  Walhovd, K.B., and Fjell, A M, “One-year test–retest reliability of auditory ERPs in young and old adults,” International Journal of Psychophysiology,46(1). 29-40. Oct.2002.
 
[35]  Lock, J., Garrett, A., Beenhakker, J., and Reiss, A.L, “Aberrant brain activation during a response inhibition task in adolescent eating disorder subtypes,” American Journal of Psychiatry. Jan.2011.
 
[36]  Han, S.D., Bangen, K.J., and Bondi, M.W, “Functional magnetic resonance imaging of compensatory neural recruitment in aging and risk for Alzheimer's disease: review and recommendations,” Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders,27(1). 1. Feb.2009.
 
[37]  Barron, E., Riby, L.M., Greer, J., and Smallwood, J, “Absorbed in thought the effect of mind wandering on the processing of relevant and irrelevant events,” Psychological science. Mar.2011.
 
[38]  Riby, L M, “The Joys of Spring: Changes in Mental Alertness and Brain Function,” Experimental psychology,60(2). 71-79. Jan.2013.
 
[39]  Polich, J., and Criado, J.R, “Neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of P3a and P3b,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60(2). 172-185. May.2006.
 
[40]  Trenkwalder, C., and Winkelmann, J, “Pathophysiology of the restless legs syndrome,” Sleep and movement disorders, Philadelphia: Butterworth/Henemann. 322-32. 2003.
 
[41]  Bokura, H., Yamaguchi, S., and Kobayashi, S, “Event-related potentials for response inhibition in Parkinson's disease,” Neuropsychologia,43(6). 967-975. 2005.
 
[42]  Frank, M.J., Moustafa, A.A., Haughey, H.M., Curran, T., and Hutchison, K.E, “Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(41). 16311-16316. Aug.2007.